Eoin Wignand, Robin Boers en Lize Boers
2024-2025
Lyceum Schravenlant
lyceumschravenlant.nl
Hugo de Grootstraat 4
3119 HA Schiedam
Engels
Darienne Harms
Eoin Wignand (6A1)
Robin Boers (6A2)
Lize Boers (5H2)
Struggling with language is universal to the human condition. It seems as though we have all struggled with language at least a few times in our lives. It is a tale as old as time: you want to convey some kind information either verbally or through writing, but you either do not know the words or you just cannot get the sentence to sound right. The former may just be human error, but the latter could be fixable.
English is renowned for being a more complicated language (Oxford Royale, 2024). Complex rules and inconsistencies, like irregular verbs, the various conflicting tenses and irregular plural, make it extremely difficult for L2 learners to get a grasp on the language. Additionally, the spelling of words in the English language often seems to indicate more about their etymology rather than their pronunciation.
Inspired by similar efforts such as Esperanto (Li, 2003) and Lojban (Nicholas, 2003), we strive to create our own constructed language that is highly regular and predictable, grammatically and morphologically consistent, easy to understand, and reduces ambiguity and misunderstandings in text-based communication. Our goal for this research is to create a complete language that can be used for every day human communication on level A1 of the Common European Framework of Reference (CEF).
In doing this, we hope to learn more about language theory and gain insight into the various linguistic concepts and tradeoffs that go into forming a language.
In our first chapter, we will explore linguistics and its various subfields to gain a better understanding of language from an academic perspective. We will be looking at examples of existing constructed languages and the various decisions and tradeoffs that they have made. Finally we will be examining ways to optimize our language for efficiency of communication.
In the second chapter, we will explain the process of constructing our language, and the various steps we have taken in doing so. Moreover, it contains a full description of the morphology and syntax of our language. We have also included an extensive appendix containing a full vocabulary list, based on frequency lists from both English and Dutch respectively.
Lastly, in the third chapter we will share our findings and discuss potential next steps and further research. Additionally, for the excited reader, we will provide various resources for the construction of your own language.
We hope you have as much fun reading this as we had writing it.
(Or read the original version or see the slides for our final presentation.)
Phonology and phonetics are studies about the sounds needed to produce spoken languages. The lines between the fields are blurry. It seems linguists have yet to reach consensus on the exact definition of the terms (Ohala, 1990). Whilst there is a lot of overlap between the fields, generally, the following distinctions can be made:
Phonetics is the study of the physical aspects of speech sounds (Cohn & Huffman, 2014), specifically the production and articulation of sounds. In phonetics, sounds are called phones and denoted using square brackets (Emrys et al., 2009) in an approximate alphabet developed by the International Phonetics Association (Delahunty & Garvey, 2010).
Phonology, however, is occupied with abstract sounds as perceived by the brain (Cohn & Huffman, 2014). Here, sounds are called phonemes and written down in between slashes (Emrys et al., 2009). The actual pronunciations—phones— for a phoneme can differ based on the surrounding phonemes in a word.
For example: the /p/ phoneme can be both [p] (in "spit") and [pʰ] (in "pit"), where the latter is pronounced with a little extra puff of air called aspiration (Emyrs, 2009). Phonological rules dictate how phones correspond to phonemes. Examples would be devoicing—where consonants are weakened at the end of words or following a voiceless sound—, and spirantization—where consonants shift "up" in between vowels (Emrys et al., 2009).
For our purposes, the exact definition of these terms and differences between the subfields does not matter all that much. However, a term that is important is the phonetic inventory of a language. The phonetic inventory describes all possible sounds that occur in a language (Emrys et al., 2009). Together, they form the phonaesthetics (sometimes referred to as sprachgefühl) of a language: the general vibe that a language conveys (Emrys et al., 2009).
Orthography studies the wide variety of ways we write languages down. The smallest unit of writing (a single symbol), is called a grapheme (Emyrs et al., 2009). The simplest writing system is the alphabet, wherein every grapheme corresponds to a single phoneme (Emrys et al., 2009).
Western alphabets are all derivatives of the Latin or Roman alphabet. A romanisation is therefore a mapping of phonemes to graphemes where you assign a letter from the Latin alphabet to every phoneme in the phonetic inventory of your language (Emyrs et al., 2009). This is by far the simplest way to write down any arbitrary language, and is thus often used by linguists for reasoning about languages.
Romanisations, like for example the Chinese romanisation Pinyin, can also act as a shortcut to write logographic languages down using standardized QWERTY keyboards (Emyrs et al., 2009).
However, alphabets are not the only possible writing system. Other cultures often utilize syllabaries or logographs for their writing. A syllabary is similar to an alphabet, but in a syllabary a grapheme corresponds to an entire syllable, rather than a single phoneme (Britannica, 1998). A prominent language using a syllabary for its orthography is Japanese. Logographic writing goes even further: a single grapheme corresponds to a whole word or grammatical construction. Examples would include Korean and Chinese.
Morphology is the study of words: how they are formed from syllables and how they can be combined to form other words (Emyrs et al., 2009).
The lexicon or vocabulary is composed of all words that appear in a language (Don et al., 2023). Words can be divided into two categories. Content words include nouns, verbs, adjectives, and adverbs, and they typically refer to things, actions, qualities, and concepts. This category is open-class: it is easily extendable with new words (Don et al., 2023). Function words, on the other hand, is a closed-class category of words which includes prepositions, conjunctions, articles, pronouns, and auxiliary verbs. They serve a grammatical function, rather than carrying semantic or lexical meaning (Don et al., 2023). In a way, the function words act as grammatical glue to bind the content words together. Thus, it is a lot harder to introduce function words than it is to introduce new content words, which is why function words are virtually never loan words (Don et al., 2023).
Content words can be further divided into categories:
A homonym refers to a word that has multiple, distinct meanings. (bark, from trees; bark, the sound a dog makes)
A synonym refers to distinct words with a shared meaning; the reverse of a homonym. (small, little, tiny as a specifiers of size)
An antonym refers to two words with obverse meanings. (light—dark, up—down, hot—cold)
A hyponym refers to a word which is a subcategory of another word. (chair is hyponymic with furniture; vodka is hyponymic with beverage)
A hypernym refers to a word that is a supercategory of another word; the reverse of a hyponym. (furniture is hypernymic with chair; beverage is hypernymic with vodka)
In morphology, words are called lexemes, which are composed of morphemes (Don et. al, 2023). According to Don et al. (2023), morphemes are the "smallest identifiable meaningful units" (p. 182) of language. Words are formed by varying linguistic processes; mainly derivation and inflection.
Derivation extends the vocabulary of a language by deriving new content words from existing ones, often using affixes (Don et al., 2023). For example:
Happy | Bake |
Unhappy | Bakery |
Happiness | Baker |
Inflection, on the other hand, changes the structure of a word to fit a grammatical construction (Don et al., 2023). For example, here are some inflections of the word "to eat":
I eat, he eats, we are eating, they have eaten.
Generally speaking, derivation creates novel words, while inflection changes the shape of existing words. Inflection is required to make sentences grammatically correct, but adds little meaning. Furthermore, derivation may change the categories of words (noun -> verb -> adjective etc.), but inflection cannot (Belfakir, 2020).
Moreover, inflection is typically also more semantically consistent than derivation is, as illustrated by Belfakir (2020):
cars 'more than one car'
tables 'more than one table'
shoes 'more than one shoe'
computerize 'put into a computer' → 'put into X'
modernize 'make (more) modern' → 'make more X'
brotherize 'provide with a brother' → 'provide with X'
Another way of forming words is compounding, where two or more existing words are stitched together to form a new word. This differs from derivation, because "[c]ompounding consists of the combination of two or more lexemes, whereas derivation is characterized by the addition of an affix, that is, a bound morpheme, to a lexeme" (Booij, 2005, p. 1). A compound word consists of a nucleus and an adjunct (Don et al., 2023). The nucleus determines the thing being named, which is also the part of the compound that gets pluralised. The adjunct further specifies the nucleus, in a way behaving similarly to an adjective (Don et al., 2023).
fietstour — fietstouren | tourfiets — tourfietsen |
kabeltv — kabeltv's | tv-kabel — tv-kabels |
English has three types of compounds: open (living room, high school), hyphenated (well-being, mind-blowing), and closed (bedroom, highway, cheesecake). In other languages, such as German and Dutch, this distinction is not made (Don et al., 2023).
Typically, compounds are hyponymic with their nucleuses (Bauer, 2008): bedroom is a subcategory of room. However, sometimes the nucleus of the compound is unrelated to the meaning of the compound itself. These kinds of compounds are called exocentric, opposed to endocentric (Don et al., 2023). Examples from English would include cutthroat, pickpocket, breakfast, and scarecrow.
There's two distinct types of plural:
Additive plural indicates that there is more than one of something. It is characterized by its referential homogeneity, that is, every member of the group 'dogs' is of type 'dog' (Overstreet & Yule, 2023). Essentially all Western-European languages feature additive (or 'regular') plural.
Associative plural, on the other hand, refers to (human) groups featuring referential heterogeneity; every member of the group is a unique individual. In addition, these individuals are all in some way related to each other, or a prominent member of the group (Overstreet & Yule, 2023). Hence, 'associative' plural. Constructions like these have been reported to be widespread in the languages of Africa, Asia, Australia, and the Pacific (Daniel & Moravcsik, 2013).
Based on their morphology, we can categorize languages into three distinct categories:
Isolating languages make little use of inflection and instead utilize supplemental words and context to convey meaning (Don et al., 2023). Their ratio of morphemes to words is often fairly low, meaning most words consist of no more than one or two morphemes. In extreme cases, there is no distinction between words and morphemes: each word is also a morpheme and vice versa.
Agglutinating languages are more morphologically complex, and make heavy use of affixes to construct long, complex and very specific words (Don et al., 2023). In agglutinative languages, words are formed by stringing together morphemes in a linear sequence. Each added affix has a distinct grammatical function that contributes to the meaning of the word (Don et al., 2023).
As an example, take the English curiosity of 'antidisestablishmentarianism', which consists of seven morphemes (anti- dis- establish -ment -ari -an -ism), all attributing to the meaning. In English, words like these are rare. However, in agglutinative languages such as Turkish they are very common.
Fusional languages are similar to agglutinating languages (in their morphological complexity), but where agglutinated words are "assembled" of distinct, easily recognisable affixes, morphemes in fusional languages fuse in a way that changes their forms (Don et al., 2023). You can think of agglutinative languages as LEGO, and fusional languages as clay.
The meaning of words can differ based on the social-cultural setting. The denotation says what the word objectively means or represents—the dictionary definition (Don et al., 2023). The connotation of the word, however, is the "emotional value, stylistic value, or culturally-defined associations evoked by the word" (Don et al., 2023), and changes over space, time and generations.
Syntax is the linguistic subfield concerned with the construction of sentences out of smaller parts (Don et al., 2023), namely phrases and (subordinate) clauses. A phrase, like a compound, consists of a nucleus and an adjunct, where the adjunct further specifies the nucleus. The nucleus can be identified by the fact that the phrase would be ungrammatical without it, while the adjunct can be left out without issues.
'very nice people'
In the phrase above, people is the nucleus and very nice the adjunct. Phrases can be nested; in the previous example, the adjunct itself is a phrase as well, where nice is the nucleus and very the adjunct ('nice people' is grammatical but 'very people' is not).
A predicate is a phrase that says something ('predicates') about the subject of the sentence. In the following example, the cat is the subject, and is at home the predicate.
'the cat is at home'
The predicate cannot exist without the subject; thus, some phrases have a dependency on other phrases, called arguments. The amount of arguments they depend on is called their valency. For example, 'to give' calls for two arguments:
'Bob gives a book' | |
'Bob gives' | (ungrammatical— what does Bob give?) |
'Gives a book' | (ungrammatical— who gives a book?) |
Some predicates are avalent: they do not require the presence of a subject or other arguments (Don et al., 2023). However, in languages such as English and Dutch, a subject is still required to make the sentence grammatical (Don et al., 2023):
'het regent' | 'it rains' |
'het is half zeven' | 'it is six thirty' |
In these cases, the subject is not a real agent, but rather a sort of placeholder, typically third person singular (Don et al., 2023). This is apparent by the fact that the sentence becomes ungrammatical if rewritten with a different subject (Don et al., 2023):
'Robin regent' | 'Eoin rains' |
'Lize is half zeven' | 'Coco is half past six' |
Phrases that are not required by the predicate are called adjuncts (Don et al., 2023). In the following example, to Alice functions as the adjunct:
'Bob gives a book to Alice'
Sometimes, a sentence is self-reflective. An example would be: 'the man washes himself'. The predicate 'to wash' is bivalent, but in the example there is just a single participant; the man both executes and undergoes the action. In these cases, a reflective pronoun ('himself') is used.
The predicate and its arguments describe the essence of a sentence, while adjuncts further specify or describe the situation. Within sentences, phrases can take on a variety of semantic roles (Mora Gutiérrez, 2001):
Agent: the 'doer' of the action denoted by the
predicate.
Patient: the 'undergoer' of the action or event denoted
by the predicate.
Benefactive: the entity that benefits from the action or
event denoted by the predicate.
Instrument: the medium by which the action or event
denoted by the predicate is carried out.
Locative: the place where the action or event denoted by
the predicate is situated.
The valency of the predicate determines how many of the semantic roles are required to be fulfilled in order to make the sentence grammatical. The sentence below illustrates all of the semantic roles above:
The artist painted a beautiful landscape for the community with spray paint on the brick wall.
In the example, 'to paint' has a valency of two, so only the artist and a beautiful landscape are strictly necessary in order for the sentence to be grammatical.
Consider the following two sentences:
Hannibal Lecter murdered two detectives.
Two detectives were murdered by Hannibal Lecter.
Semantically, the sentences are equivalent. In both sentences, Hannibal Lecter is the agent, and two detectives the patient. However, the grammatical roles fulfilled by the phrases differ. The grammatical roles mainly decide the perspective the situation is described from (Don et al., 2023). In the first sentence Hannibal Lecter is the subject, whilst in the second sentence two detectives is.
For active sentences, the semantic roles map to grammatical roles roughly like this (Don et al., 2023):
Agent -> Subject
Predicate -> Verb
Patient -> Object
Benefactive -> Indirect object
The order in which grammatical roles appear is called the word order. Both Dutch as well as English require subject-verb-object (SVO) for declarative sentences. However, other languages may require a different word order (Don et al., 2023):
Furthermore, languages may have an unfixed word order, meaning the language does not prescribe a specific word order; all combinations of subject-verb-object are considered grammatical. An example would be Finnish, which has a semi-unfixed word order.
In many languages, including English and Dutch, the word order is dependent on the sentence type:
Declarative: 'the cat is at home' (SVO)
Imperative: 'be home, cat!' (VOS)
Interrogative: 'is the cat home?' (VSO)
Most languages spoken around the world are naturalistic languages. A naturalistic language is a language that has evolved through centuries of use (Cambridge Dictionary, n.d.). Opposite to this, are languages that are purposefully built by individuals. Constructed languages, also called conlangs, can be created for real-world or fictional speakers. The practice of creating new languages is called conlanging (Schreyer, 2021).
According to Emyrs et al. (2009), constructed languages can be categorised into several groups:
Posteriori languages are based on an existing language while in contrast, priori languages are completely made from scratch. Priori languages are seen as harder to create, because it is difficult to make something completely novel without accidentally including influences of existing languages (Emrys et al., 2009).
A naming language, where you only need phonology and morphology, is relatively easy to create in comparison to a more extensive language, allowing actual phrases and sentences. In a more extensive language, you need to take syntax into account, as well as a more comprehensive morphology. When you decide to create a fully speakable language, pragmatics and stylistics require more attention and you will need a larger vocabulary (Emrys et al., 2009).
When creating a language, most conlangers decide on some kind of 'hook', to make their language unique. For example, Logban does not allow ambiguity and Kēlen does not make use of verbs. Ithkuil also has an interesting hook, as it attempts to pack as much information as possible in the smallest space (Emrys et al., 2009).
For our research, we studied the following conlangs in order to possibly learn from their decisions and design processes. We will briefly introduce each language and summarise their features below.
Esperanto, the most well-known conlang, is a typical auxlang. It was constructed to be a neutral language, meant for international communication. Since it is no one's mother tongue, no one is at a disadvantage when communicating with each other (Li, 2003).
One of the design features of Esperanto is simplicity in its linguistics. It has a simple phonological system, with a highly regular stress pattern. Therefore, it has an easy pronunciation. Esperanto's spelling is completely phonetic, with one grapheme per phoneme (Li, 2003). It also has a consistent morphology, which makes it attractive for new learners, because it reduces the number of words needed to understand and produce a new text (Gledhill, 1998). Its word order is unfixed, although SVO occurs the most often (Parkvall, 2010).
Esperanto is primarily based on European languages, like Russian, French, Spanish, German and English. Like many European languages, it also has a large number of colour terms, distinguishes between 'he' and 'she', it lacks an associative plural and it has a comparative particle 'ol'. Another profoundly European feature is the use of a relative pronoun in a sentence; 'the boy who waved turned out to be Robin.' Esperanto uses 'kiu' (Parkvall, 2010).
If you are already familiar with European languages, learning Esperanto can be easier. From our own experiences, we know that while learning Esperanto you are able to recognise a considerable amount of words, which profoundly speeds up the learning process.
Lojban is a loglang that has been in creation for over four decades, led by The Logical Language Group. It is based on the six most spoken languages and designed to be culturally neutral.
Lojban was built to be clear of any ambiguity. It does not have any distinct, separate words that sound alike, nor does it have words that have more than one unrelated meanings (homonyms). The design of Lojban is that the function of every word is conclusively clear. Lojban is unique because it is completely free of any exceptions in spelling, word formation (morphology), grammar rules and pronunciation (Nicholas, 2003).
Lojban makes use of the Roman alphabet, only leaving out h, q and w. Furthermore, it uses the apostrophe ('), representing a sound similar to /h/, as well as the period (.), as part of the morphology, indicating pauses in the sentence, and the comma (,) to indicate a syllable break within a word. The written language correlates exactly with the spoken language, thanks to its phonetic spelling (Nicholas, 2003).
Lojbans grammar is based on the principles of logic, using a system with a relationship and arguments (Nicholas, 2003). In order to understand these grammar rules, one has to have a lot of metalinguistic knowledge, for example, you need to understand the principles of predicate logic.
The simplicity of these grammar rules make the language easier to learn for newcomers, although it does require a different approach. While the languages we are used to use verbs and nouns, Lojban does not have an exact matching concept, meaning that newcomers might have a bit of a hard time understanding what is going on.
When comparing Esperanto and Lojban, the biggest difference is the languages they are based on. While Esperanto is primarily based on European languages and features, Lojban also incorporates Chinese, Hindi and Arabic influences. Lojban's grammar features also accommodates structures from non-European languages. The vowel pairs and the power of the spoken language also corresponds more to Slavic languages. Another difference is the initial purpose. While Esperanto was specifically designed as an auxiliary language for neutral international communication, Lojban was designed instead as a linguistic tool for studying and understanding language (Nicholas, 2003).
Toki pona is an engelang, literally translating to 'language good/simple' (Emrys et al., 2009). In contrast to Lojban, Toki Pona makes heavy use of ambiguity. A multitude of words can have multiple meanings, making the language largely context based. Another aspect of this ambiguity is that it can not specify whether a word is singular or plural. Toki Pona only has fourteen letters in its alphabet and just 131 root words. Due to its minimal vocabulary, the language makes use of the combination of words. For example, 'friend' is made of the combination 'good person' ('jan pona') (Pona, 2005).
When comparing Toki Pona and Esperanto, the main difference is the vocabulary. In Esperanto, one needs to learn a large number of words before being able to hold a conversation or read a text, while in Toki Pona, you start with only 131 root words. The vocabulary in Toki Pona is the easy part, the skill of combining these words to create others is the real challenge.
Utility of language is important to consider. According to Jaeger & Tily (2010) "language utility can be understood as relative to a human language user's communicative needs." This implies that the function a language serves must align with the user's communicative needs so that it can be properly utilised by said user. This also implies that if, for example, our constructed language does not align with our communicative needs, it will not serve a purpose to us and we will not be able to properly utilise it. Furthermore, communication succeeds when a message can be conveyed from one person to another without, or only slightly, diverging from what the first person wants to say. Usually, getting the point across takes a lot of effort from both parties. However, successful communication can be achieved with minimal effort. This is what is called communicative efficiency (Gibson et al., 2019). Communicative efficiency is part of our communicative needs and can therefore be linked to the utility of the language.
However, our need for informativeness in language seems to contradict our general preference for efficiency and its simplicity. Language has to be informative: we need to convey information as accurately as possible, but it must also be simple: conveying said information should require minimal effort. An informative language should in theory be complex and nuanced, thus completely disregarding the need for efficiency (Kemp et al., 2018).
Although, languages seem to balance both informativeness and simplicity (Gibson et al., 2019). According to Niklaus et al. (2019) there are two main ways to successfully reduce linguistic complexity while maintaining the information that needs to be conveyed: lexical simplification and syntactic simplification.
Lexical simplification refers to the substitution of difficult words or phrases with words that are easier to comprehend but retain the information (Niklaus et al., 2019).
According to Jaeger et al. (2010), it has been observed that words that are more predictable due to the preceding context add less information to sentences. They claim that our need for communicative efficiency makes us pronounce those predictable words with less duration than the information-holding unpredictable words.
Furthermore, predictable words are not only pronounced with shorter duration, they also do not get enunciated properly. The words that contain information are the words that get pronounced properly and with longer duration (Jaeger & Tily, 2010).
Syntactic simplification is the process "of reducing the grammatical complexity of a text, while retaining its information content and meaning" (Siddharthan, 2004).
According to Niklaus et al. (2019), a specific kind of syntactic simplification is sentence splitting, which takes a sentence and divides it into several shorter and simpler sentences that then become easier to process. They observed that it does make information easier to understand, but on the other hand the sequences of shorter sentences lose the cohesion and longer texts become harder to interpret. This can be remedied by linking sentences with cue words (Niklaus et al., 2019).
Another way they remedied this problem is by distinguishing core information from contextual information (Niklaus et al., 2019). This way we can use ambiguity to our advantage. By leaving out information that is given in context, we can speak more efficiently (Gibson et al., 2019).
Another thing to note for syntactic simplicity is dependency syntax and dependency locality. Dependency syntax is defined as the order individual words must have in order to derive the meaning of the sentence they are in. Dependency locality is the idea that the distance between the words that are linked by dependency should be as short as possible, as minimising dependency length increases communicative efficiency (Gibson et al., 2019).
The CEF is a document published by the Council of Europe, which aims to accurately describe "the linguistic means needed to communicate efficiently with speakers in another language" (Goullier, 2007), by quantising language proficiency on a common scale of levels, providing common terminology across languages and educational contexts, and introducing "a common methodology for analysing and describing situations" (Goullier, 2007).
The document is not prescriptive in nature, but rather intended as a tool for describing linguistic competence (Goullier, 2007). The six levels of proficiency, as defined by the CEF, are (Council of Europe, 2020):
In this research, we will use these levels as a guideline for the complexity of the grammar and richness of the vocabulary when creating our language. We will focus on levels A1 and A2 specifically, which are defined as follows (Council of Europe, 2020):
A1 | Can understand and use familiar everyday expressions and very basic phrases aimed at the satisfaction of needs of a concrete type. Can introduce him/herself and others and can ask and answer questions about personal details such as where he/she lives, people he/she knows and things he/she has. Can interact in a simple way provided the other person talks slowly and clearly and is prepared to help. |
A2 | Can understand sentences and frequently used expressions related to areas of most immediate relevance (e.g. very basic personal and family information, shopping, local geography, employment). Can communicate in simple and routine tasks requiring a simple and direct exchange of information on familiar and routine matters. Can describe in simple terms aspects of his/her background, immediate environment and matters in areas of immediate need. |
Additionally, the CEF includes a list of illustrative descriptors in the form of 'Can Do' statements for each level (Council of Europe, 2020). Since this paper fundamentally focuses on simplification of vocabulary and grammar, we are most interested in the 'reading' and 'writing' categories, which are defined as follows (Council of Europe, 2020):
Reading | Writing | |
---|---|---|
A1 | I can understand familiar names, words and very simple sentences, for example on notices and posts or in catalogues. | I can write a short, simple postcard, for example sending holiday greetings. I can fill in forms with personal details, for example entering my name, nationality and address on a hotel registration form. |
A2 | I can read very short, simple texts. I can find specific, predictable information in simple everyday material such as advertisements, prospectuses, menus and timetables and I can understand short simple personal letters. | I can write short, simple notes and messages relating to matters in areas of immediate need. I can write a very simple personal letter, for example thanking someone for something. |
Our main goal is making a language that is complete, consistent, and lowers the bar to effective and successful communication. Furthermore, we want to enrich the orthography of our language in order to convey information typically exclusively communicated via speech, thus preventing miscommunications via text.
Before starting on the process of actually constructing our language, we need to decide how extensive and complete we want the language to be. As an example, a naming language is far less complex than a more advanced language meant for full human communication (Emrys et al., 2009).
We want our language to be complete enough to write stories in and have moderately complicated conversations. This means we need to create a comprehensive morphology and complete syntax and grammar, rather than constructing only the phonology and orthography.
At the bare minimum, we want to enable communication on level A1 of the Common European Framework of Reference for Language skills (CEF).
To gain a better understanding of what others generally find difficult or lacking in their own language, we decided to make a survey. This survey consists of questions concerning what people may find difficult in the languages they are currently trying to learn, what they may be missing in their native language(s), and what they like in their native language(s). We are using Google Forms to create this survey and we have sent it via email to (most of) our school.
The problem we are trying to solve with this design is the irregularity and inconsistency of natural languages, which seem to be more exception than rule. We aim to create a language that is complete and consistent, thus being easier to learn and understand.
Our audience consists of people in the range of 11-40 years old, looking to learn a new language. As noted, we sent out a survey to research what the background of our audience is. From the results of that survey (see Appendix 3), we have concluded that our target audience is primarily Dutch-English bilingual.
Constraints are the limited amount of time and resources
we have available during this school year.
Existing solutions would include Esperanto and Lojban, as
discussed in the previous chapter.
The heavy use of ambiguity in Toki Pona is interesting, but not something we would like to adopt. The complete removal of ambiguity Lojban has accomplished is equally interesting, but a computer-like grammar is not something we aim for either. Esperanto, on the other hand, is quite similar in scope and design philosophy to our language. Furthermore, since Esperanto is primarily based on languages we speak, there might be significant overlap between Esperanto and our language as well.
Actors and stakeholders in the design process would be us as language designers, people learning the language, and people who might be interested in teaching the language.
The first step in creating our language was pinning the phonology down. In this stage, we tried to answer the questions:
We based our phonology on those of English, with some influences from Dutch, as those are the languages we speak and thus are most familiar with. Most of the decisions we made in this phase were either improvements for pronunciation and general 'speakability' of the language, as well as personal stylistic choices.
Stylistic choices:
During the development of the language, we have not found a case where we have needed to utilise phonetic rules in order to ensure pronounceability, but cases may of course arise if the language were ever really spoken.
Vowels | Consonants | ||
---|---|---|---|
(phone) | (grapheme) | (phone) | (grapheme) |
[æ] (cat) | æ (U) | [d] (dick) | d |
[a] (schaap) | ɑ (a) | [p] (peel) | p |
[ɑ] (car) | ʌ (V) | [m] (mouse) | m |
[ɔ] (offer) | ɔ (c) | [n] (nugget) | n |
[o] (ghost) | o | [t] (twat) | t |
[ə] (cut) | e | [ɦ] (house) | h |
[ɪ] (if) | i | [s] (sick) | s |
[i] (fantasy) | y | [ʂ] (shit) | ȿ (z) |
[e] (day) | λ (7) | [t͡ʃ] (chocolate) | x |
[ɛ] (shrek) | э (3) | [g] (gay) | g |
[aj] (bite) | Σ (Z) | [b] (boobs) | b |
[u] (poops) | θ (0) | [f] (fuck) | f |
[aʊ] (pow) | σ (q) | [v] (virgin) | v |
[y] (schuur) | u | [l] (lol) | ʟ (l) |
[ʋ] (what) | w | ||
[j] (you) | ɥ (j) | ||
[k] (can't) | k | ||
[ɻ] (run) | r |
The way phonemes are combined in syllables largely decides the phonaesthetics of the language, because the way consonants and vowels are arranged can change the sound and perception of the language (Emrys et al., 2009).
Our language's syllables adhere to the (C)V(C) structure. This is fairly common in (constructed) languages. The consonants are both optional, but a single vowel cannot be a syllable. Meaning, 'by', 'yb', and 'byb' are all valid syllables, but 'y' is not. We decided to keep the rules simple, and thus not allow consonant combinations like 'tr…' and '...ng', as they would unnecessarily complicate the phonotactics of the language.
After pinning the phonology down, we need a way to write our language down. Questions in this stage are:
We want our language to be purely phonetic. That means a single grapheme for every phoneme and vice versa. We decided to base our alphabet on the Latin alphabet, for the simple reason that it is familiar and the easiest to type for our target audience (see Appendix 3). Where possible, we retained the existing letters from the English and Dutch alphabets respectively, and in many cases, they even aligned with the IPA notation as well.
Since our alphabet is phonetic, we naturally needed to extend the traditional Latin alphabet to accommodate for additional vowels (which are typically represented by a combination of graphemes rather than a single grapheme). In these cases, we grabbed letters from various existing alphabets, such as the Greek and Russian alphabets. In doing this, we purely looked at the shapes of the graphemes and what 'felt right'; we did not look at the original pronunciation in the respective languages, although that would have been a good idea in hindsight.
Another important aspect of our orthography is the fact that we do not make the distinction between upper and lower case, since we think it does not serve any semantical purpose other than 'looking nice', yet unnecessarily complicates the language. In this aspect, our orthography is similar to that of Toki Pona, which also does not distinguish between upper and lower case (in their romanisation).
In the table above, the letters in parentheses are a romanisation of our (sometimes exotic) alphabet. These are used for typing and transmitting the alphabet in standard ASCII, which can then be transformed into the proper Unicode symbols by a special piece of software, which is available on our website. This is similar to Pinyin, a Chinese romanisation used for easily writing Chinese on iPhones (Emyrs et al., 2009).
Since our orthography requires glyphs that are not typically provided by traditional Western fonts, we might eventually look into creating our own font for rendering our language as well. For the time being, please pick a font with appropriate Unicode support, such as Inter (sans-serif) or Junicode (serif).
After much experimentation, we settled on a morphology that is somewhere between isolating and agglutinating. Fusional was ruled out entirely from the beginning, because, although it looks nice, it does not align with our goal of a regular, predictable language.
Our language features a decently extensive derivational system with a fair variety of affixes, which is an exemplary property of an agglutinative morphology. However, our words are short, typically only a few syllables long, and in many cases we use compounding and adjectives rather than word derivation to convey complex semantics.
English is our ancestral language, similar how many words in contemporary Euroasian languages derive from classic Latin. We decided on English as opposed to Dutch because English is the field of research this paper is applicable to. For laying the groundwork of the vocabulary, we utilized frequency lists from English and Dutch respectively.
Additionally, we made sure to include categories explicitly mentioned in the CEF. We also incorporated words people feel are missing in their own language, according to the survey (see results in Appendix 3).
In Appendix 1 we provide a complete word list with English and Dutch translations of a variety of categories consisting of the most commonly used words and phrases in both Dutch and English respectively.
We are using a consistent list of derivational affixes in order to further extend the vocabulary. Suffixes are used for changing a verb into a noun, in accordance with the rules below. All other derivational affixes will be prefixes, in an attempt to limit the complexity of the language.
noun | suffix | in English |
---|---|---|
verb -> action | -a | -ion, -ment, -al, -ance |
verb -> agent | -er | -er |
verb -> patient | -y | -ee |
verb -> benefactive | -yp | non-existent |
verb -> past part. | -o | -ed |
verb -> cont. part. | -ono | -ing |
The following table contains prefixes that change the meaning of words, but not the word category. Consider this list to be non-exhaustive, as it might be further extended with additional derivational prefixes in later publications.
effect | prefix | in English |
---|---|---|
negation | ne- | im-, in-, de-, anti-, a- |
without | na- | -less |
with | an- | -ful |
state/condition | kʌd- | -ness, -y |
similarity | ko- | -like |
manner | no affix | -ly, -ily |
moment | a- | non-existent |
repetition | rλ- | re- |
Compounds are formed by putting multiple existing words together to form a new word. In our language, all compounds are hyphenated, and follow the structure of adjunct-nucleus, where the adjunct is a specifier for the nucleus. In virtually all cases, the compound is hyponymic with its nucleus.
For pluralisation, append an -s if possible in accordance to the language's morphology, otherwise append -es instead.
Loan words are not allowed in our language. Instead, we derive the word from an existing word, or create an entirely new word. The sole exception to this rule is names. We do not want to translate names, whether it be people, cities, places or products. Instead, authors should write the name down in its original language, and then italicize or underscore it.
Prefer italicization over underlining; underscoring is only allowed to accommodate for paper writing, because it "is known to reduce the readability of text significantly, as it changes the word shape and interferes with descenders, letters that drop below the line like p, q and j" (Obendorf & Weinreich, 2003).
Our language does not support forming diminutives by appending suffixes, like Dutch does. Instead, to indicate something is small, use the adjective 'ʟiʟ'. Similarly, using the adjective 'wɔp' functions as an augmentative.
Our sentences follow a semi-unfixed word order. The basis of the sentence is structured according to SVO, where the subject can optionally be dropped when implied by the verb conjugation (more on that later). Any additional phrases can be placed at any point in the sentence, but must be preceded by a grammatical 'glue' word, to indicate its grammatical role.
grammatical role | keyword |
---|---|
benefactive | vɔr |
instrument | mэt |
locative | эt |
time | ɔm |
nested sentence | ɥy |
The preposition 'эt' is used for generic localities, where you would usually utilize 'at' in English. If necessary, the locale can be further specified by replacing this preposition with one of the words from the 'Localities' table from Appendix 1. The same applies for the preposition 'ɔm' as well, which can optionally be replaced with the prepositions 'dΣmɑ' and 'dΣmy'.
In total, our language contains three types of pronouns:
Our verbs are conjugated by applying the suffix in the right most column to the end of the verb. The vowel utilized in the chosen suffix indicates the tense of the verb, where 'e' indicates the present, 'ʌ' the past, and 'ɔ' the future. To conjugate a past participle or adjective, append -o instead.
Our language is pro-drop. That means, that in sentences where the (subjective) pronoun can be inferred from the verb conjugation, it may be entirely omitted. For example, the sentence 'mu ʌrem' ('I am') may be simplified to just 'ʌrem', because the -em suffix already implies first person.
1st person | mu | -em |
---|---|---|
2nd person | dy | -ed |
3rd person | ʟλ | -eʟ |
Group 1st (in) | wy | -ep |
Group 2nd (out) | jθ | -ek |
Group 3rd | vΣ | -ev |
+n for possessive +sэv for self-reflective |
e = present ʌ = past ɔ = future |
For avalent verbs, we utilize the generic third person pronoun and suffix.
All sentences are assumed to be declarative unless specified otherwise. To change the sentence type to an interrogative or imperative sentence, prepend one of the following specifiers. These are required to be the very first word of the sentence, preceding every other phrase, in order to indicate sentence type upfront, and thus prevent miscommunications.
interrogative | da + SENTENCE |
---|---|
imperative | xa + SENTENCE |
In imperative sentences, verbs are left unconjugated. Thus, instead of appending the usual suffix, append just the phoneme indicating tense (e, ɔ, ʌ).
In cases where sentences are nested (subordinate clauses, modal verbs etc.), we utilize the word 'ɥy,' which indicates that the following phrase will in fact be a nested sentence. A few examples:
wisem ɥy kэneʟ ne ɥy ʟσfeʟ.
(know-I) (nested sentence) (can-he) (not) (nested sentence) (walk-he)
'I know that he can not walk.'
poremyseʟ ɥy gλheʟ ɑsɑ.
(promise-he) (nested sentence) (go-he) (there)
'He promised to go there.'
Since nested sentences can include grammatical 'glue' words, the nested sentence has to be the last phrase of the sentence, to prevent ambiguity. This has the added benefit that it makes sentences easier to understand, because it minimizes the cognitive distance, allowing the main idea to be processed first and then linearly presenting additional information, rather than having the reader jump around the sentence to understand it.
Nested sentences can also include further nested sentences, again utilizing the 'ɥy' word, given they are the last phrase of the nested clause. However, this makes for longer, more complex sentences and is therefore discouraged.
pθveʟ ɥy sædeʟ ɥy ʟuveʟ de kɔʟɔr nэna.
(allowed-he) (nested sentence) (say-he) (nested sentence) (love-he) (the) (color) (pink)
'He is allowed to say that he likes the color pink.'
For direct speech, we utilize rules similar to those from English and Dutch, placing the literal sentence in quotation marks. The writer is free to choose whatever quotation marks they prefer, but are encouraged to stay consistent within their writing.
Robin says: "I like the color pink."
sæded Robin "ʟuvem de kɔʟɔr nэna."
For indirect speech, we reuse the existing grammar already present for nested sentences:
Robin says that he likes the color pink.
sæded Robin ɥy ʟuveʟ de kɔʟɔr nэna.
Possessive pronouns are formed by appending -n to the respective objective pronoun, as discussed in the section 'Pronouns & verb conjugation.' However, if you want to indicate an possessive for an entire phrase, use the external genitive 'vɔn':
OBJECT vɔn OWNER
This leaves us with two ways to indicate possession in our language:
Periphrastic possessive | Possessive determiner |
---|---|
de lybre vɔn de Σmy 'The man's book' |
mun lybre 'My book' |
Constructions such as 'de lybre vɔn mu' are considered ungrammatical.
Adjectives are put after the noun they are further specifying. The order in which they appear is irrelevant, unless it affects the semantics of the word in question. For example, a 'Σmʌn ʟiʟ wɔp' means something akin to 'big dwarf,' while 'Σmʌn wɔp ʟiʟ' means 'small giant.'
Negations and affirmations, like 'not', 'wel', 'often', 'never', 'always' are, in our language, seen as "adjectives" that modify the meaning of a verb, and thus follow the same rules that apply for adjectives too. They are placed right behind the verb.
English | Dutch | Our language |
---|---|---|
Good | Goed | bɔn |
Better | Beter | bɔnbɔn |
Best | Best | bɔnbɔnbɔn |
This also leaves room for creative figurative speech, such as 'bɔnbɔnbɔnbɔn' ('bestest').
To use this in a sentence, we utilize the preposition 'dʌn', which literally translates to 'than'. For example:
bɔnbɔn dʌn bɔnbɔnbɔn
'better than best'
For equalities, we utilize the preposition 'eku', which translates to 'as … as …'. For example:
wɔp eku dʌt bowɑ
(big) (equals) (that) (building)
'as big as that building'
Tone indicators contextualise a sentence by indicating the (emotional under)tone of the sentence, or indicating references (such as inside jokes) or quotes. They are typically placed at the very end of the sentence.
Tone | Translation | Indicator |
---|---|---|
joking | ɥukono | ɥuk. |
sarcastic | sʌrkʌso | sʌr. |
serious/genuine | sirλus | sir. |
positive | pɔso | pɔs. |
negative | sebo | seb. |
citation/quote | sytɑto | syt. |
reference | refэro | ref. |
These can also be inverted by prepending them with an n-. They are used as follows:
Oh, great, another meeting. I just love them so much [sʌr.]
No, seriously, we need to finish this today [nɥuk.]
If we do not change anything now, we have a real problem [sir.]
I really do appreciate your help, thank you [nsʌr.]
Ik heb daar geen actieve herinnering aan [ref.]
Misschien is hij op zoek naar een functie elders [ref.]
"God does not play dice" — "Stop telling God what to do" [syt.]
Yeah, sure, I'm just thrilled to be stuck here all day [sʌr.]
This reminds me of that time you tried to bake cookies and nearly burned the house down [nɥuk.]
Tone indicators are exclusively used in writing, and are not meant to be pronounced explicitly (but can be used by a reader to infer intonation).
The following table contains a helicopter-view of the decisions we have made in the various stages of construction, all with the goal to simplify the syntax while retaining semantics.
Linguistic aspect | Options | ||
---|---|---|---|
Orthography | Alphabet | Syllabary | Logographic |
Morphology | Isolating | Agglutinative | Fusional |
Pluralisation | Irregular | Affix ('s, -en) | Vowel change |
Word order | SOV | SVO | Unfixed |
Compounds | Open | Closed | Hyphenated |
Possessive | External genitive | Genetative affix ('s) | Determiner (adjective) |
Degr. of comparison | Reduplication | Affix (-er, -st) | Vowel change |
In order to test our language we have translated excerpts from various (short) stories, which can be found in Appendix 2.
Other than that, another way to test the language would be teaching it to friends or relatives. However, due to shortage of time, we decided to focus on the translations instead.
Our goal was to create a complete, consistent language from scratch for spoken and written communication. If we take a look at our original design constraints from the program of requirements:
Our grammar and morphology adheres to every single one of the requirements.
Furthermore, we specified we wanted the language to be usable for A1 level communication. As mentioned in Chapter 1, this entails "everyday expressions and very basic phrases aimed at the satisfaction of needs of a concrete type." Our vocabulary and syntax more than covers this requirement.
Therefore, we must conclude that we have created a complete, usable language. As noted in the previous chapter, Appendix 2 contains translations of excerpts from multiple (short) stories.
Overall, the process of making this language was surprisingly easy. We did lots of work upfront during the theoretical research, which made executing the language quite straightforward. Furthermore, deciding on the fundamental structure of the language was pretty simple as well, as we could make these decisions together easily. The most challenging part was creating the vocabulary, because of the sheer volume of necessary words (big!) Maintaining morphological consistency was a challenge too, as was deciding on certain syntactic structures; we insufficiently researched these topics beforehand.
We still personally struggle with the pronunciation of our own language. For that reason, we've also built a tool that can perfectly pronounce our language, which is possible because the phonology is entirely phonetic. The tool is available on our website.
As for our future plans, we noticed that our language is wonderfully concise. On average, sentences in our language are significantly shorter than their equivalents in English and Dutch respectively. This makes our language a perfect fit for quickly writing down great amounts of information, such as taking notes in class, which is something we'd like to try out. Other ideas would include creating a Duolingo-like app for learning our language, creating our own font for rendering the orthography—potentially utilizing ligatures—, creating a Google Translate-like app based on the OpenAI API, and teaching our language to other people. Additionally, further extending the vocabulary and introducing additional syntactic elements would possibly enable B1 level communication.
In conclusion, we created a fully usable language, learned a lot about linguistics & language theory, and had a lot of fun doing it.
You might, after reading all this, be wondering what the name of our language is. It is called Σkɔnik (pronounced 'iconic'), which might already have been apparent from the title of this document. It doesn't translate to anything; it does not mean anything either. It just sounds cool.
There's a few things we could have done differently:
Bauer, L. (2008). English Exocentric Compounds. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-008-9122-5
Belfakir, L. (2020). Morphology: Derivation vs. Inflection. https://fldm.usmba.ac.ma/uploads/2020/03/AN423-Belfakir-Intro-2-Lx-8-Morphology-2.pdf. (archived)
Booij, G. (2005). Compounding and derivation: evidence for Construction Morphology. https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.264.08boo
Britannica. (1998, July 20). Citation. In Encyclopedia Britannica. Retrieved February 14, 2025, from https://www.britannica.com/topic/syllabary
Cambridge Dictionary. (n.d.). Citation. In Cambridge online dictionary. Retrieved February 14, 2025, from https://dictionary.cambridge.org/dictionary/english/natural-language
Cohn, A. & Huffman, M. K. (2014). Interface between phonology and phonetics. Oxford Bibliographies. https://doi.org/10.1093/obo/9780199772810-0168
Council of Europe. (2020). Common European Framework of Reference for Languages: Learning, teaching, assessment — Companion volume. Council of Europe Publishing, Strasbourg. https://coe.int/lang-cefr
Daniel, M., & Moravcsik, E. (2013). The associative plural. In M. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures (WALS) (pp. 150-153). Oxford University Press.
David C.S. LI (2003). Between English and Esperanto: what does it take to be a world language? International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 2003(164), 33-63. https://doi.org/10.1515/ijsl.2003.055
Delahunty, G. & Garvey J. (2010). The English Language: From Sound to Sense. https://doi.org/10.37514/per-b.2010.2331
Don, J., Meyer, C. & Rispens, J. (2023). Taal en Taalwetenschap. (3rd ed.). Wiley Blackwell.
Emrys, S. Fink, A. & Peterson, D. (2009). Conlanging 101. Language Creation Society. Retrieved September 8, 2024, from https://conlang.org/cl101.pdf. (archived)
Gibson, E., Futrell, R., Piantadosi, S. T., Dautriche, I., Mahowald, K., Bergen, L., & Levy, R. P. (2019, February 23). How Efficiency Shapes Human Language, TICS 2019. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/w5m38
Gledhill, C. (1998). The grammar of Esperanto (pp. 150-pp). Lincom Europa. https://u-paris.hal.science/hal-01220008/document
Goullier, F. (2007). Council of Europe tools for language teaching: Common European Framework of Reference and Portfolios. Les éditions Didier. https://www.coe.int/en/web/common-european-framework-reference-languages/documents
Henning, J. (1995). A naming language. https://fiatlingua.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/10/fl-00003E-00.pdf. (archived)
Kemp, C., Xu, Y., & Regier, T. (2018, January 14). Semantic typology and efficient communication. https://escholarship.org/content/qt1126n182/qt1126n182.pdf
Kisa, S. E. (2003). Oou: the Insane Language (or Friendly Genocide). Retrieved September 17, 2024, from https://drive.google.com/file/d/1jLxGsmLDXHxxJJkB1PaAaoDvLHz-tLDY/view. (archived)
Mora Gutiérrez, J. P. (2001). Directed motion in English and Spanish. Estudios De Lingüística Del Español, 11. http://elies.rediris.es/elies11/cap5111.htm
Nicholas, N., & Cowan, J. W. (Eds.). (2003). What Is Lojban? Logical Language Group. https://www.lojban.org/static/publications/level0/lojbanLevel0.paperback.pdf
Niklaus, C., Cetto, M., Freitas, A., & Handschuh, S. (2019). Transforming Complex Sentences into a Semantic Hierarchy. https://arxiv.org/pdf/1906.01038
Obendorf, H., & Weinreich, H. (2003). Comparing link marker visualization techniques: changes in reading behavior. Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on World Wide Web, 736—745. Presented at the Budapest, Hungary. https://doi.org/10.1145/775152.775255
Ohala, J. (1990). There is no interface between phonology and phonetics. Journal of Phonetics, 18(2), 153—171. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0095-4470(19)30399-7
Overstreet, M., & Yule, G. (2023). Associative plural marking in English varieties. English Today, 40(2), 122—126. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0266078423000354
Oxford Royale. (2020, September 11). Why is English so hard to learn? Oxford Royale Academy. https://www.oxford-royale.com/articles/learning-english-hard
Pona, T. (2005). the language of good—the simple way of life. https://www.academia.edu/download/32488651/toki-pona-lessons.pdf
Pona, T. (2024). Toki Pona as a Small World Language. https://tokipona.org/small_world_language.html
Schreyer, C. (2021). Constructed languages. Annual Review of Anthropology, 50(1), 327—344. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-anthro-101819-110152
Siddharthan, A. (2006). Syntactic simplification and text cohesion. Research on Language and Computation, 4(1), 77—109. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11168-006-9011-1
Jaeger, T. F., & Tily, H. (2010). On language 'utility': processing complexity and communicative efficiency. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science, 2(3), 323—335. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.126
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Hello | Hallo | hλ, hΣ, ɥo |
World | Wereld | ωθd |
Bye | Doei | bΣ, xσ |
Hello (formal) | Hallo (formeel), goedendag | bɔnsɔʟ |
Bye (formal) | Doei (formeel) | bɔnmэr |
And | En | эn |
Or | Of | ɔf |
The | De, het | de |
A/an | Een | en |
Yes | Ja | ɥæ |
- | Wel | ɥe |
No | Nee | no |
Not, none | Niet, geen | ne |
With | Met | ɑn |
Without | Zonder | nɑ |
Good | Goed | bɔn |
Bad | Slecht | ωɔm |
Life | Leven | vyvɑ |
Death | Dood | dod |
Love | Liefde | ʟuv |
Hate | Haat | hλt |
Light | Licht | usɔʟ |
Free | Vrij | otσ |
God | God | gɔt |
Devil | Duivel | duveʟ |
Thing | Ding | ɑfэro |
Human, person | Mens, persoon | Σmʌn |
Woman, girl | Vrouw, meisje | Σmɑ |
Man, boy | Man, jongen | Σmy |
Self | Zelf | sэv |
Sentience/self-awareness | Zelfbewustzijn | sentɑʟo |
Conscience | Geweten | bonæro |
Intrusive thoughts | Opdringerige gedachten | ωomæro |
Much/many | Veel | din |
Few | Weinig | fin |
More | Meer | dindin |
Less | Minder | finfin |
Everything | Alles | ʌʟ |
Nothing | Niks, niets | niʟ |
Everyone | Iedereen | ʌʟmʌn |
Nobody | Niemand | niʟmʌn |
Always, forever | Altijd | ʌʟdΣm |
Often, usually | Vaak | dindΣm |
Sometimes | Soms | findΣm |
Never | Nooit | niʟdΣm |
Exactly | Precies | dinɑ |
Almost | Bijna | finɑ |
But, however | Maar | ɥэt |
Also, furthermore, moreover | Ook, daarnaast | ʌʟso |
Because, as | Omdat | kʌs |
Therefore | Daarom, dus | dʌs |
If | Als | if |
Whether | Of | ȿu |
Than | Dan | dʌn |
This | Dit, deze | dλs |
That | Die, dat | dʌt |
Here | Hier | ɑsy |
There | Daar | ɑsɑ |
Next | Volgende | tɔm |
Previous | Vorige | ɥэn |
Begin | Begin | kɔmo |
Mid | Mid | mid |
End | Eind | эnde |
Like | Zoals | ʟyko |
Complete, full, finished, saturated | Compleet, volledig, 'af', verzadigd | ʌpo |
Norm, rule | Norm, regel | nɔrmu |
Value (moral) | Waarde (moreel) | vɑʟu |
Value (capital) | Waarde (kapitaal) | fundλ |
Normal, usual | Normaal, gewoonlijk | nɔrmuto |
Abnormal, unusual | Abnormaal, ongewoonlijk | nenɔrmuto |
Extraordinary | Bijzonder | |
Same | Zelfde | sɑmo |
Different, other | Anders | nesɑmo |
Known, familiar | Bekend | fʌmʟɑ |
Unknown, strange | Onbekend, vreemd | nefʌmʟɑ |
Enjoyable | Lekker | ɥem |
Unpleasant | Vies | eω |
Fast | Snel | ɥyt |
Slow | Langzaam | neɥyt |
Cool | Cool | Σsy |
Beauty, beautiful | Mooi | bo |
Ugly | Lelijk | fugo |
Fancy | Fancy | fænȿy |
Clean | Schoon | ȿebo |
Dirty | Vies, smerig | ȿefugo |
Important | Belangrijk | impo |
Irrelevant | Onbelangrijk | neimpo |
Big, tall | Groot, lang | ωɔp |
Small | Klein | ʟiʟ |
Young, new | Jong, nieuw | ɥuno |
Old | Oud | odu |
Alone | Alleen | ʟono |
Together | Samen | эnsɑmo |
Pain | Pijn | pλn |
Pleasure | Lust | lusɑ |
Magic | Magie | mэxik |
Meaning | Betekenis | signyfo |
Steady, consistent | Consistent | kɔnsэkɑ |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Name | Naam | nɔm |
Home | Thuis | hom |
Work | Werk | ωσko |
City | Stad | stэte |
Building, house | Gebouw, woning | bσωɑ |
Country | Land | lʌnde |
Voyage | Reis | voɥɑ |
Fact | Feit | fэkto |
Opinion | Mening | mλnuno |
Deep | Diep(gang) | dypfo |
Joke | Grap | ɥuko |
Joking | Grappend | ɥukono |
Serious | Serieus | sirλus |
Quote | Citaat | sytɑto |
Reference | Referentie | refэro |
Free | Gratis | fyby |
Profit | Winst | fyt |
Cheap | Goedkoop | xypu |
Problem | Probleem | pэkɑ |
Music | Muziek | musyk |
Song | Nummer, liedje | kʌnto |
Piano | Piano | pɑno |
Guitar | Gitaar | gytɑr |
Artist | Artiest | ɑrtyk |
Bag | Tas | sʌko |
Ball | Bal | bʌʟ |
Hobby | Hobby | hɔby |
Sport | Sport | эspɔrtɑ |
Bed | Bed | bэd |
Room | Kamer | rθm |
Garden | Tuin | kurɑ |
Glasses | Bril | oku-vytro |
Door/port | Deur, poort | pɔrtu |
Wall | Muur | ωɔl |
Window | Raam | ωindoω |
Corner | Hoek | hθk |
Hole | Gat | ȿʌt |
Book | Boek | lybre |
Notebook | Schrift | ryt-lybre |
Pen | Pen | lydo |
Vehicle | Voertuig | tokɑ |
Car | Auto | motokɑ |
Train | Trein | tλn |
Bike | Fiets | bΣk |
Plane | Vliegtuig | æro-tokɑ |
Chair | Stoel | sэgθ |
Table | Tafel | tɑblu |
Desk | Bureau | ωσk-tɑbʟu |
Bank | Bank | bʌnku |
Cushion, pillow | Kussen | pilθω |
Basket | Mand | mɑnde |
Bowl | Kom | boωʟe |
Shelf | Plank | ȿævo |
Closet, cupboard, case | Kast | ȿævos |
School | School | ekɔl |
Class | Klas | kɑlʌs |
Classroom | Lokaal | kɑlʌs-rθm |
Open | Open | open |
Closed | Dicht, gesloten | syto |
Reaction | Reactie | rλʌktɑ |
Allergy | Allergie | ωɔmrλʌktɑ |
Sick | Ziek | syk |
Disease | Ziekte | kʌdsyk |
Healthy | Gezond | sʌntλ |
Health | Gezondheid | kʌdsʌntλ |
Store | Winkel | ȿɔp |
Supermarket | Supermarkt | ωɔp-ȿɔp |
Photograph/picture | Foto | foto |
Movie/video | Beeld/video | vydo |
Sound | Geluid | fono |
Smell | Geur | odɔr |
Trans- (across, spanning) | Trans- (over) | tʌn |
Cis- (within) | Cis- (binnen) | sis |
Tele- (over distance) | Tele- (op afstand) | tλʟe |
Television | Televisie | tλʟe-vydo |
Telephone | Telefoon | tλʟe-fono |
Radio | Radio | tλʟe-musyk |
Camera | Camera | xymy |
Screen | Scherm | ȿin-vytro |
Machine | Machine | mɑxine |
Computer | Computer | kʌʟkuʟɑ |
Tool | Gereedschap | tulo |
Screw | Schroef | rum |
Pan | Pan | pɔt |
Party | Feest | fэd |
Pool, bath | Zwembad | ɑkωɑ-ȿʌt |
Present | Cadeau | kɑdo |
Treasure | Schat | ȿɑsoxu |
Row | Rij | roω |
Power | Kracht | pσer |
- | Jarig | ɥipy |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
How are you (doing)? | Hoe gaat het? | dɑ ʌred kɑ? |
Sorry | Sorry | sɔry |
Thank you | Dankjewel | xʌno |
You're welcome | Alsjeblieft (gevend) | nepэkɑ |
Of course | Natuurlijk | |
Please | Alsjeblieft (vragend) | ωys |
(For) example | (Bij)voorbeeld | эksΣmu |
Bless you | Gezondheid | bɔnsʌntλ |
Enjoy your meal | Eet smakelijk | bɔnэskɑ |
Good job! | Goed gedaan! | bɔntθno |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
What | Wat | ku |
Where | Waar | kσ |
Why | Waarom | ko |
How | Hoe | kɑ |
When | Wanneer | kΣ |
Which (pronoun) | Welke (vnw) | kλ |
Who (subject) | Wie (onderwerp) | ky |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Well | Nou | nθ |
Oops | Oeps | ωθpes |
Unfortunately | Helaas | ȿΣdes |
In fact | Eigenlijk | ɑfэkto |
- | Oei | θɥ |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Up | - | up |
Down | - | dσω |
Left | Links | ʟэv |
Right | Rechts | rΣt |
Above | Boven | bovder |
Underneath | Onder | under |
In front | Voor | vɔrder |
Behind | Achter | hinder |
Next to | Naast | nэsder |
(In) between | Tussen | ωisder |
In(side) | In/binnen | inder |
Out(side) | Uit/buiten | Σder |
Atop | Op (rakend boven) | bovtσȿ |
Against | Tegen (naast rakend) | nэstσȿ |
Through(out) | Door(heen) | vydɔr |
Into | In (bew.) | vyinder |
Out of | Uit (bew.) | vyΣder |
Over | Overheen (bew.) | vybovder |
From | Van(af), van(uit) | vyɑsy |
To | Naar(toe) | vyɑsɑ |
Up (mov.) | Omhoog | vyup |
Down (mov.) | Omlaag, naar beneden | vydσω |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
From (time) | Van(af) (tijd) | dΣmy |
To (time), until | Tot (tijd) | dΣmɑ |
About | Over | uber |
Between | Tussen | tikse |
(One) of | (Een) van | us |
For (reason) | Voor (reden) | fʌsɑ |
Both | Beide, allebei | buder |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
North | Noord | nɔren |
East | Oost | Σsde |
South | Zuid | sude |
West | West | ωэsde |
0 | niʟ |
1 | θn |
2 | by |
3 | xy |
4 | dэkɑ |
5 | py |
6 | эsɑ |
7 | sid |
8 | ɔkdɑ |
9 | nik |
10 | dys |
20 | bydin |
30 | xydin |
40 | dэdin |
50 | pydin |
60 | эsdin |
70 | sidin |
80 | ɔkdin |
90 | ninin |
100 | cэt |
1000 | kin |
1.000.000 | miʟ |
1.000.000.000 | gig |
Infinity | ʌʟdin |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
-st, -nd, -rd | -ste | эd- |
First | Eerste | эdθn |
Second | Tweede | эdby |
Third | Derde | эdxy |
... | ... | ... |
Last | Laatste | эdʟʌs |
Random ('a') | Willekeurig ('een') | эdrʌn |
For constructions like 'two thirds', we utilize the word 'pэr', which means something akin 'one over'. So in our language 'two thirds' would be 'by pэrxy'.
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
To count | Tellen | kʌʟ- |
Plus / to add | Plus / optellen | pɔs- |
Minus / to subtract | Min / aftrekken | seb- |
Times / to multiply | Keer / vermenigvuldigen | tΣm- |
Over / to divide | Over / delen | dλv- |
To equal | Is gelijk aan | eku- |
Positive | Positief | pɔso |
Negative | Negatief | sebo |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Language | Taal | ʟʌnge |
Alphabet | Alfabet | ʌʟfabэt |
Letter | Letter | ʟэter |
Sentence | Zin | sine |
Text | Tekst | tэku |
Word | Woord | ωɔrde |
Question | Vraag | fʌrɑ |
Answer | Antwoord | pʌrɑ |
Letter | Brief | ȿɔt |
Story, history | Verhaal, geschiedenis | histɔru |
Translation | Vertaling | tʌn-ʟʌnge |
Conversation | Gesprek | kɔnverɑ |
Discussion | Discussie | diskesɑ |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Time | Tijd | dΣm |
Millenium | Millenium | kinɥir |
Century | Eeuw | cэtɥir |
Decade | Decennium | dysɥir |
Year | Jaar | ɥir |
Quarter (of a year) | Kwartaal | xyʟθn |
Month | Maand | ʟθn |
Week | Week | ωik |
Day | Dag | sɔʟ |
Night | Nacht | mэr |
Hour | Uur | σr |
Minute | Minuut | minut |
Second | Seconde | sэk |
Clock | Klok | ʟɔko |
Present | Heden | nσ |
Past | Verleden | tσω |
Future | Toekomst | futσ |
Morning | Ochtend | kɔmosɔʟ |
Afternoon | Middag | midsɔʟ |
Evening | Avond | omэr (abbr. of kɔmomэr) |
Midnight | Middernacht | midmэr |
At day | Overdag | ɑsɔʟ |
At night | 's nachts | ɑmэr |
Now | Nu | ɑnσ |
Then (past), earlier, ago | Toen, eerder, geleden | ɑtσω |
Then (future), later, soon | Dan, later, binnekort | ɑfutσ |
Before | Voordat | ɑkɔmo |
During, while | Gedurende, tijdens, terwijl | ɑmid |
After | Nadat | ɑnde |
Today | Vandaag | dλssɔʟ |
Yesterday | Gisteren | tɔmsɔʟ |
Tomorrow | Morgen | ɥэnsɔʟ |
Weekend | Weekend | ωikэnde |
Birthday | Verjaardag | ɥipy-sɔʟ |
Again | Opnieuw | rλ |
Still | Nog steeds | ɑturo |
Anymore | Niet meer |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Spring | Lente | xin |
Summer | Zommer | sɔmɑ |
Autumn | Herfst | ʌdem |
Winter | Winter | ωindɑ |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
March | Maart | rλnuɑry |
April | April | pʌperɑry |
May | Mei | mɑɥoɑry |
June | Juni | ʟysuɑry |
July | Juli | hʌrmuɑry |
August | Augustus | rɔbuɑry |
September | September | sidɑry |
October | Oktober | okduɑry |
November | November | ninuɑry |
December | December | oduɑry |
January | Januari | ɥunoɑry |
February | Februari | oωuɑry |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Wednesday | Woensdag | sɔʟθn |
Thursday | Donderdag | sɔʟby |
Friday | Vrijdag | sɔʟxy |
Saturday | Zaterdag | sɔʟdэkɑ |
Sunday | Zondag | sɔʟpy |
Monday | Maandag | sɔʟэsɑ |
Tuesday | Dinsdag | sɔʟsid |
(While we were at it, we also once and for all resolved whether the week starts on Monday or Sunday—in our language it starts on Wednesday, take it or leave it.)
Angry | Angry | ɑrɑgo |
Annoyed | yrɑto | |
Frustrated | rusɑto | |
Jealous | ɥɑʟono | |
Sad | Saddened | deprэso / mэh |
Depressed | ||
Stressed | sэtvo | |
Alone / lonely | ʟono | |
Fear | Fear(ful) | sɑkɑro |
Insecure | nesэkuro | |
Anxious | ʌxono | |
Shy | ȿΣ | |
Joy | Happy | bΣto |
In love | ʟuvo | |
Excited | eksyto | |
Confident | sэkuro | |
Blissfully unaware, illusioned | ʟusyno | |
Calm | kʌʟmo | |
Disgust | Disgusted | disgusto |
Embarrassed | embɑrʌso | |
Sarcastic | sʌrkʌso | |
Surprise | Surprised | sepryso |
In awe | ʌωo | |
Disillusioned | neʟusyno | |
Curious | kuro | |
'Bad' | Bored | bɔro |
Tired | tΣro | |
Indifferent | nekæro | |
Delusional | deʟuʟo |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Vision/sight | Zicht | vysɑ |
Hearing | Gehoor | hɔrɑ |
Smell | Reuk | rukɑ |
Taste | Smaak | tэsɑ |
Touch | Tast | tσȿɑ |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Color | Kleur | kɔʟɔr |
White | Wit | ʟide |
Black | Zwart | omʌȿ |
Grey | Grijs | ʟidʌȿ |
Light | Licht | sɔʟɑ |
Dark | Donker | mэrɑ |
Red | Rood | rɔȿ |
Yellow | Geel | ɥэʟoω |
Blue | Blauw | ʟθω |
Oranje | Orange | orɑn |
Green | Groen | rθn |
Purple | Paars | pэrɑ |
Pink | Roze | nэnɑ |
Shiny | Glimmend | ȿin |
It is possible to derive colors by combining them. Here is a non-exhaustive list of examples:
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Turquoise | Turkoois | ʟθω-rθn |
Light blue | Lichtblauω | sɔʟɑ-ʟθω |
Brown (aka dark orange) | Bruin (aka donkeroranje) | mэrɑ-orɑn |
Gold | Goud | ȿin-ɥэʟoω |
Silver | Zilver | ȿin-ʟidʌȿ |
Bronze | Brons | ȿin-orɑn |
(These are compounds, hence the hyphen :)
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Family | Familie | fʌmʟy |
Brother | Broer | nybo |
Sister | Zus | sis |
Sibling | - | sinyno |
Mother, mom | Moeder, mama | ʌmɑ |
Father, dad | Vader, papa | ɑdɑ |
Parent | Ouder | udэrɑ |
Grandma | Oma | omɑ |
Grandpa | Opa | odɑ |
Grandparent | Grootouder | odudэrɑ |
Daughter | Dochter | dotɑ |
Son | Zoon | sono |
Child | Kind | sydo |
Wife | Vrouw | mun Σmɑ |
Husband | Man | mun Σmy |
Fiance | Verloofde | xiku |
Partner | Partner | |
Girlfriend | Vriendinnetje | |
Boyfriend | Vriendje | |
Far- | Achter- | hin- |
Step- | Stief- | tyf- |
Aunt | Tante | hin-ʌmɑ |
Uncle | Oom | hin-ɑdɑ |
(gender neutral) | (genderneutraal) | hin-uderɑ |
Niece | Nicht | hin-sis |
Nephew | Neef | hin-nybo |
Cousin | (genderneutraal) | hin-sinyno |
Note about in-laws (schoon- in Dutch): they are utterly confusing. Just say what you mean. 'My girlfriend's parents' is so much clearer than 'parents in-law' or whatever. Also, what the actual f*ck does 'zwager' mean?? Same goes for step- etc.
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Baby | Baby | ʟiʟ-kido |
Child | Kind | kido |
Teenager | Tiener | dyno |
Adult | Volwassene | ɑdo |
Friend | Vriend(in) | ɑmy |
Enemy | Vijand | σnomy |
Attacker, abuser | Aanvaller, misbruiker | σωer |
Victim | Slachtoffer | σωy |
Witness | Toeschouwer | vyser |
Defender, protector | Verdediger | potэker |
Player | Speler | toɥer |
Fan | Fan | fæn |
Crowd | Menigte | Σmʌndin |
Team | Team | toɥers |
Teammate | Teamgenoot | toɥ-ɑmy |
President | President/premiër | impo-bɔser |
Teacher | Docent/leraar | tyxer |
Student | Student | tyxy |
Pupil | Leerling | ʟλrer |
Classmate | Klasgenoot | kɑlʌs-ɑmy |
Lawyer | Advocaat | ʟʌω-potэker |
Patient | Patient | rλsʌnty |
Waiter, server | Ober, serveerder | эskɑ-giver |
Priest, preacher | Priester, dominee, predikant | ɥohʌner |
Philosopher | Filosoof | |
Police (officer) | Politie(agent) | poʟyser |
Firefighter | Brandweerman | fΣrσωer |
Doctor | Dokter | mэdiker |
Artist | Artiest | mλker |
Employer | Werkgever | ωσk-giver |
Employee | Werknemer | ωσk-givee, ωσk-nλmer |
Employee | Medewerker | ωσker |
Boss | Baas | bɔser |
Peasant | Onderdaan, boerenpummel | bɔsy |
Vampire | Vampier | væmpir |
Reporter | Verslaggever | rypoter |
Journalist | Journalist | |
Poet | Dichter | poet |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Body | Lichaam | bɔdy |
Head | Hoofd | hæd |
Brain/mind | Brein | mΣnde |
Face | Gezicht | fλsu |
Neck | Nek | nэk |
Beard | Baard | bærdo |
Stomach | Buik | buk |
Arm | Arm | ʌrme |
Shoulder | Schouder | ȿɔʟdɑ |
Leg | Been | bλne |
Knee | Knie | ny |
Eye | Oog | oku |
Tear | Traan | kyrɑkωɑ |
Mouth | Mond | ɔro |
Lip | Lip | ʟip |
Tongue | Tong | tɔnge |
Nose | Neus | nλs |
Ear | Oor | σro |
Hair | Haar | ωig |
Hand | Hand | hʌnde |
Foot | Voet | fθte |
Finger | Vinger | finge |
Toe | Teen | tθne |
Nail | Nagel | nλgeʟ |
Tooth | Tand | dэn |
Heart | Hart | kʌrdu |
Blood | Bloed | vʌsku |
Sweat | Zweet | sʌωɑku |
Bone | Bot | bone |
Muscle | Spier | mesku |
Voice | Stem | vokɑ |
Skin | Huid | pэʟis |
Wing | Vleugel | fugэʟ |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Wood | Hout | ωot |
Paper | Papier | pɑpyres |
Glass | Glas | vytro |
Earth | Aarde | эrde |
Lead | Lood | ʟэd |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Animal | Dier | tyr |
Pig | Varken | nɔr |
Cow | Koe | mθ |
Horse | Paard | ɔs |
Cat | Kat | mσ |
Dog | Hond | ωθf |
Donkey | Ezel | dɔnkλ |
Chicken | Kip | tɔk |
Rooster | Haan | |
Giraffe | Giraffe | rʌfλ |
Elephant | Olifant | ɔʟy |
Lion | Leeuw | ʟyɔn |
Tiger | Tijger | tΣger |
Hippo | Nijlpaard | hipo |
Fish | Vis | obʟib |
Shark | Haai | ȿɑk |
Duck | Eend | duken |
Fly | Vlieg | fʟugy |
Frog | Kikker | fɔger |
Mouse | Muis | mus |
Monkey | Aap | mɔnkλ |
Bird | Vogel | birden |
Cockroach | Kakkerlak | kukɑrɑxɑ |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Clothes, clothing | Kleding(stuk) | vэstθ |
Dress, robe | Jurk | |
Suit | Pak | fænȿy-vэstθ |
Shirt | Shirt | sэtɑ |
Jeans/pants | Broek | xɑmy |
Shoe | Schoen | bθt |
Sock | Sok | sɔk |
Coat, jacket | Jas | ɥɑs |
Underwear | Ondergoed | under-vэstθ |
Sweater, hoody | Trui | hθdy |
Outfit | Outfit | xɑmysэtɑ |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Vegetable | Groente (enkelv.) | rθnes |
Potato | Aardappel | potʌto |
Tomato | Tomaat | tomʌto |
Broccoli | Broccoli | rθn-bɔkɔʟy |
Cauliflower | Bloemkool | ʟidʌȿ-bɔkɔʟy |
Carrot | Wortel | kɑrɔt |
karctan | Boon | kэbyn |
Corn | Mais | mΣȿy |
Eggplant | Aubergine | Σgpyʌntɑ |
Courgette | Courgette | |
Cucumber | Komkommer | kθmkθmer |
Garlic | Knoflook | gɑʟik |
Advocado | Advocado | ʌdvokɑdo |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Fruit | Fruit (enkelv.) | rɔȿes |
Apple | Appel | æpeʟ |
Banana | Banaan | bɑnɑnɑ |
Pear | Peer | pir |
Grape | Druif | duf |
Berry | Bes | bэr |
Strawberry | Aardbei | rɔȿ-bэr |
Blueberry | Bosbes | pэrɑ-bэr |
Mandarine | Mandarijn | orɑn-bэr |
Orange | Sinaasappel | |
Lemon | Citroen | ɥэʟow-bэr |
Lime | Limoen | rθn-bэr |
Pineapple | Ananas | ʌnɑnʌs |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Drink | Drinken (enkelv.) | dinke |
Water | Water | ɑkωɑ |
Milk | Melk | ʟʌkto |
Coffee | Koffie | kɔfλ |
Tea | Thee | ty |
Soda | Frisdrank | sukrλ-dinke |
Hot chocolate | Chocolademelk | xokoʟɑ-ʟʌkto |
(Alcoholic) drink | (Alcoholhoudende) drank | ɑʟko-dinke |
Cocktail | Cocktail | ɑʟko-ȿɑpɥe |
Juice | Sap | ȿɑpɥe |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Sweet | Zoet | suto |
Salty | Zout | sσto |
Sour | Zuur | sσro |
Bitter | Bitter | bitɑ |
Spicy | Heet/pittig | hæt |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Knife | Mes | poker |
Fork | Vork | fɔk |
Spoon | Lepel | espun |
Plate | Bord | pɑto |
Cup | Beker | kɔpy |
Bottle | Fles | budeʟ |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Food | Eten | эskɑ |
Candy | Snoep (enkelv.) | kænty |
Meat | Vlees | vyɑnde |
Breakfast | Ontbijt | kɔmo-эskɑ |
Lunch | Lunch | mid-эskɑ |
Dinner | Avondeten | omэskɑ (contr. of omэr+eskɑ) |
Dish | Gerecht/maaltijd | эskɑko |
Rice | Rijst | rΣs |
Soup | Soep | sθp |
Salad | Salade | sʌʟɑdem |
Bread | Brood | pæn |
Sugar | Suiker | sukrλ |
Salt | Zout | sэʟ |
Pepper | Peper | pэpэr |
Cheese | Kaas | fɔrmɑgy |
Egg | Ei | Σg |
Cookie | Koekje | bisky |
Pancakes | Pannekoeken | pʌnkλky |
Chocolate | Chocolade | xokoʟɑ |
Icecream | IJs | sukrэm |
Waffle | Wafel | rɔbin |
Donut | Donut | donet |
Cake | Taart, cake | kλky |
Fries | Patat, friet | pʌtʌt |
Burger | Burger | burber |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Nature | Natuur | nɑtu |
World | Wereld | ωθd |
Earth | Aarde | |
Tree | Boom | bom |
Forest | Bos | bomdin |
Root | Wortel | rθt |
Sun | Zon | sɔʟ |
Moon | Maan | ʟθn |
Wet | Nat | nλɥo |
Dry | Droog | rλɥo |
Sky | Lucht | æro |
Air | Lucht | |
Wind | Wind | vэntu |
Fire | Vuur | fΣr |
Plant | Plant | pyʌntɑ |
Flower | Bloem | fyʟɔrɑ |
Grass | Gras | gyrɑsɑ |
Sand | Zand | sæn |
Dirt | Aarde/grond | humus |
Water | Water | ɑkωɑ |
Desert | Woestijn | sændin |
Beach | Strand | sænɑkωɑ |
Sea | Zee | (= body of water) ɑkωɑdin |
Ocean | Oceaan | |
Lake | Meer | |
River | Rivier | |
Rain | Regen | ɑkωɑdσω |
Wave | Golf | ωλve |
Ice | IJs | Σs |
Heat/warmth | Warm | ωɑm |
Cold | Koud | neωɑm |
Hill | Heuvel | ʟiʟ-mσtэn |
Mountain | Berg | mσtэn |
Valley | Vallei | væʟis |
Star | Ster | ʟiʟ-sɔʟ |
Snow | Sneeuw | эsno |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Religion | Religie | beʟyfɑ |
Afterlife | Hiernamaals | ɑnde-dod |
Medicine | Medicijn | medixin |
Drug | Drug | эdrɔgɑ |
Money | Geld | mɔny |
Currency | Munteenheid | kerэnsy |
Bill | Rekening | biʟ |
Price | Prijs | kɔstэ |
Contract | Contract | kontʌktus |
Marriage | Huwelijk | hΣrɑt |
Wedding | Bruiloft | |
Relationship | Relatie | kɔnэkto |
Ethnicity | Etniciteit | эtnike |
Sex | Seks | seksa |
Gender | Gender | xэndэr |
Murder | Moord | homosyde |
Crime | Misdaad | fθɥ |
Prison | Gevangenis | fθɥ-bσωɑ |
Gun | Geweer | pistɔʟ |
News | Nieuws | ɥunos, rypot |
Technology | Technologie | tekno |
Energy | Energie | эnэrxy |
War | Oorlog | ωʌr |
Peace | Vrede | pys |
Military | Militair | miʟytэr |
Game | Spel | ludo |
Ball | Bal | bʌʟ |
Science | Wetenschap | syэnse |
Sign | Bord | signe |
Magazine | Tijdschrift | mɑgɑsyn |
Election | Verkiezing | impo-ȿɑsyra |
Race | Race | rλs |
Law | Wet | ʟʌω |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
To be | Zijn | ʌr- |
To have | Hebben | hэb- |
To become | Worden | (= to be in the future, so ʌrɔ...) |
To be allowed to | Mogen | pθv- |
To be able / can | Kunnen | kэn- |
To have to | Moeten | mus- |
Should | Zouden moeten | ȿθd- |
Will / shall / to be going to | Zullen | ȿʌʟ- |
To want | Willen | ωɔʟ- |
To show | Laten zien | ȿoω- |
To make, to create, to construct | Maken | mλk- |
To do | Doen | tθn- |
To use | Gebruiken | ɥθs- |
To happen | Gebeuren | hэp- |
To move | Bewegen | vyd- |
To travel | Reizen | voɥɑv- |
To wait | Wachten | ωλd- |
To resemble | Lijken (op) | ʟyk- |
To work | Werken | ωσk- |
To like | Leuk vinden | ʟΣk- |
To love | Houden van | ʟuv- |
To miss | Missen | mys- |
To joke | Grappen maken | ɥuk- |
To care | Geven om, belangrijk vinden | kær- |
To think | Denken, vinden | pэn- |
To find | Vinden | find- |
To lose | Verliezen | ʟθs- |
To rediscover | Terugvinden | rλfind-, rλdiskɔv- |
To see/watch | Zien, kijken | vys- |
To hear | Horen | hɔr- |
To smell | Ruiken | ruk- |
To taste | Proeven | tэs- |
To touch | Aanraken/betasten | tσȿ- |
To say/talk | Zeggen/praten | sæd- |
To promise | Beloven | poremys- |
To listen | Luisteren | ʟisen- |
To feel | Voelen | sэns- |
To come | Komen | kɔm- |
To go (somewhere) | (Heen) gaan | gλh- |
To stay | Blijven | эstλh- |
To walk | Lopen | ʟσf- |
To run | Rennen | ɥytʟσf- |
To take | Nemen | nλm- |
To bring | Brengen | tэk- |
To thank | (Be)danken | xʌn- |
To understand | Snappen | ɔmpʌnd- |
To begin/start | Beginnen | kɔm- |
To end | Eindigen/stoppen | эnd- |
To know | Weten | ωis- |
To change | Veranderen | ȿʌns- |
To live (in general) | Leven | vyv- |
To live (in a house) | Wonen | hɑbyt- |
To sleep | Slapen | næp- |
To ask | Vragen | fʌr- |
To answer | (Be)antwoorden | pʌr- |
To act | Handelen | ʌkt- |
To react | Reageren | rλʌkt- |
To give/bring (to) | Geven/brengen (aan) | giv- |
To receive | Krijgen | gэt- |
To get/fetch/summon | Ophalen/(op)eisen | ɑkymon- |
To put | Plaatsen/neerleggen | ʟug- |
To choose | (Uit)kiezen | ȿɑsyr- |
To look (for) | Zoeken | suk- |
To open | Openen | open- |
To believe | Geloven | beʟyf- |
To finish/complete | Afmaken | ʌp- |
To aggravate | Boos maken | ɑrɑg- |
To annoy | Irriteren | yrɑt- |
To frustrate | Frustreren | rusɑt- |
To make jealous | Jaloers maken | jɑʟon- |
To depress | Depressiveren | deprэs- |
To stress | Stressen | sэtv- |
To isolate | Isoleren | ʟon- |
To scare | Bang maken/laten schrikken | sɑkɑr- |
To make anxious | Ongerust maken | ʌxon- |
To make happy | Blij maken | bΣt- |
To excite | Enthousiast | eksyt- |
To calm | Kalmeren | kʌʟm- |
To validate | Valideren | sэkur- |
To disgust | Tot walging brengen | disgust- |
To embarrass | In verlegenheid brengen | embɑrʌs- |
To surprise | Verrassen | seprys- |
To admire | Bewonderen | ʌw- |
To make curious | Belangstelling opwekken | kur- |
To shield | Afschermen/beschermen | ʟusyn- |
To bore | Vervelen | bɔr- |
To tire | Moe maken | tΣr- |
To deceive (yourself) | (Jezelf) om de tuin leiden | deʟuʟ- |
To deceive (someone else) | (Een ander) om de tuin leiden | gæsʟΣt- |
To gaslight | Gaslighten | |
To bleed | Bloeden | vʌsk- |
To sweat | Zweten | sʌωɑk- |
To eat | Eten | эs- |
To cook | Koken | эskɑk- |
To drink | Drinken | dink- |
To sit | Zitten | (= to be somewhere, so use ʌr- instead) |
To stand | Staan | |
To lay | Liggen | |
To hug | Knuffel | heg- |
To light, to turn on | Aansteken, aanzetten | ʟΣt- |
To force | Dwingen | fɔrx- |
To drive | (Auto) rijden | mov- |
To cycle | Fietsen | sΣkɔʟ- |
To build | Bouwen | bσω- |
To exercise | Sporten | эspɔrt- |
To meet | Ontmoeten | vθs- |
To write | Schrijven | ryt- |
To read | Lezen | ʟys- |
To attack/hurt | Pijn doen | σω- |
To defend | Verdedigen | potэk- |
To play | Spelen | toɥ- |
To learn | Leren | ʟλr- |
To teach | tyx- | |
To preach | Prediken | ɥohʌn- |
To philosophize | Filosoferen | |
To heal | Helen/genezen | mэdik- |
To police | - | poʟys- |
To fight fire | - | fΣrσω- |
To boss (around) | De baas spelen | bɔs- |
To report | Verslag geven | rypot- |
To fail | mislukken | fλʟ- |
To spread, to scatter | Verspreiden | sɑkɑt- |
To break | Kapot maken | bɔrok- |
To wear | Dragen | ωær- |
To invite | Uitnodigen | invyt- |
To face | - | fλs- |
To knit | Breien | niten- |
To sew | Naaien | ȿuω- |
To converse | Praten | kɔnver- |
To discuss (argumentative) | Discussieren | diskes- |
To join (together) | Samenvoegen | ɥon- |
To fuck | Geslachtsgemeenschap bedrijven | seks- |
To cry | Huilen | kyr- |
To dry | (Af)drogen | rλɥ- |
To wet | Natmaken | nλɥ- |
To stab | Steken | pok- |
To screw | Schroeven | rumb- |
To explore | Ontdekken (zoeken) | enkɔv- |
To discover | Ontdekken (vinden) | diskɔv- |
To chew | Kauwen | xer- |
To shine | Stralen | ȿin- |
To fly | Vliegen | fʟugeʟ- |
To connect | Verbinden | konэkt- |
English | Dutch | Σkɔnik |
---|---|---|
Fuck | Fuck | fʌk, fuk |
Bitch | Bitch | bix |
Het was een bijzondere dag en Ming wilde zijn beste vriend Tibbe een cadeau geven. Maar wat moest het worden? Een etensbak had hij al. Hij had al een mand en een kauwspeeltje. Hij had alles al. Ming piekerde zich suf. Wat geef je aan iemand die alles al heeft? Niets! Wat is er beter dan niets? Maar waar vind je niets in een wereld vol met iets? Ming hoorde Frank vaak zeggen dat er niets op tv was. Maar voor zover hij het kon bekijken was er altijd wel iets op tv. Ming hoorde Suzie en haar vriendinnen zeggen dat ze niets te doen hadden. Maar voor zover hij het kon bekijken was iedereen altijd wel iets aan het doen. [...truncated]
ʌreʟ en sɔʟ nenɔrmuto эn Ming wɔʟeʟ ɥy giveʟ en kado vɔr ʟλn amy bɔnbɔnbɔn nɔmo Tibbe. da ȿasyreʟ? hэbeʟ en eskɑ-boʟe. hэbeʟ en mɑnde эn en xerɑ-ludo. hэbeʟ ʌʟ. Ming pэneʟ din. dɑ gived ku vɔr en Σmʌn hэbo ʌʟ. niʟ! dɑ ʌreʟ ku bɔnbɔn dʌn niʟ? dɑ kσ findeʟ niʟ inder en wθd ʌpo ɑfэros? Ming hɔreʟ ɥy Frank sædeʟ ɥy ʌreʟ niʟ inder de tλʟe-vydo. ɥэt, vyseʟ ʌʟdΣm ɑfэros inder de tλʟe-vydo. Ming hɔreʟ ɥy Suzie эn ʟλn ɑmys sædeʟ hэbeʟ niʟ ɥy tθneʟ. ɥэt, ʌʟmʌn tθneʟ ʌʟdΣm ɑfэros.
These two very old people are the father and mother of Mr Bucket. Their names are Grandpa Joe and Grandma Josephine. And these two very old people are the father and mother of Mrs Bucket. Their names are Grandpa George and Grandma Georgina. This is Mr Bucket. This is Mrs Bucket. Mr and Mrs Bucket have a small boy whose name is Charlie Bucket. This is Charlie. How d'you do? And how d'you do? And how d'you again? He is pleased to meet you. The whole of this family – the six grown-ups (count them) and little Charlie Bucket – live together in a small wooden house on the edge of a great town. [...truncated]
dλses Σmʌnes din odu ʌrev de ɑdɑ эn de ʌmɑ vɔn Σmy Bucket. vΣn nɔmes ʌrev odɑ Joe эn omɑ Josephine. эn dλses Σmʌnes din odu ʌrev de ɑdɑ эn de ʌmɑ vɔn Σmɑ Bucket. vΣn nɔmes ʌrev odɑ George эn omɑ Georgina. dλs ʌreʟ Σmy Bucket. dλs ʌreʟ Σmɑ Bucket. Σmy эn Σmɑ hэbev en Σmy ʟiʟ nɔmo Charlie Bucket. dλs ʌreʟ Charlie. dɑ ʌrem kɑ? эn, dɑ ʌrem kɑ? эn rλ, dɑ ʌrem kɑ? ʌred bΣto ɥy vθsem dy. de fʌmʟy ʌpo – de эsɑ ɑdos (xɑ kʌʟe vΣ) эn de Charlie Bucket ʟiʟ – hɑbytev эnsɑmo inder en bσwɑ wot ʟiʟ nэstσȿ en stэte wɔp.
One quiet afternoon, I was pulled from the shelf where dozens like me
were arranged in neat rows.
en kʌʟmo midsɔʟ, ʌrʌm ȿɑsyro vyɑsy de ȿævo. ɑsɑ din ʟyko mu ʌrʌv inder
rowes bo.
(a) (calm) (afternoon), (was-I) (chosen) (from-mov.) (the) (shelf).
(there) (much) (resembling) (me) (was-them) (in) (rows)
(beautiful).
I traveled in the cart throughout the whole store and at the end,
placed in a small paper bag.
voɥɑvem inder de tokɑ vydɔr de ȿɔp ʌpo эn эt эnde, ʌrɔm ʟugo inder en sʌko
ʟiʟ pɑpyres.
(travel-I) (in-loc.) (the) (vehicle) (trough-mov.) (the) (store)
(complete) (and) (loc.) (end), (be-I-fut.) (placed) (in) (a) (bag)
(small) (paper).
I was brought home to a room that would soon be my entire world.
ʌrʌm nλmo vyɑsɑ hom vyɑsɑ en rθm. dλs rθm ʌrɔʟ mun wθd ʌpo.
(was-I) (taken) (there-mov) (home) (there-mov) (a) (room). (this)
(room) (be-he-fut.)(my) (complete) (world).
They screwed me into my socket and my warm glow came to life.
rumbev mu vyinder mun ȿʌt эn mun ȿin-usɔʟ wɑm kɔmʌʟ ɥy vyveʟ.
(screw-they) (me) (in-mov.) (my) (hole) (and) (my) (shiny-light) (warm)
(began-he) (nested sentence) (live-he).
My light filled the room, this room full of potential.
mun usɔʟ ʌpeʟ de rθm, dλs rθm ʌpo an futσ bɔn.
(my) (light) (fill-he) (the) (room), (this) (room) (full) (with)
(future) (good).
The bookshelves were filled, a dozen papers were spread out on the
desk, on the couch the cushions were just slightly off center and in the
corner a big, comfortable chair was positioned to face the window.
de lybre-ȿævos ʌrʌv ʌpo, pɑpyres din ʌrʌv sɑkɑto bovtσȿ de wσk-tɑbʟu.
bovtσȿ de bʌnku, de pilθwes ʌrʌv nemid ʟiʟ. inder de hθk en sэgθ wɔp bɔn
ʌrʌʟ fλso de window.
(the) (bookshelves) (were-they) (full), (papers) (many) (were-they)
(spread) (atop) (the) (work-table). (atop) (the) (couch), (the)
(cushions) (were-they) (not-mid) (small). (in-loc.) (the (corner) (a)
(chair) (big) (good) (was-it) (face) (the) (window).
This room belonged to a cheerful family.
dλs rθm ʌrʌʟ vɔn en fʌmʟy bΣto.
(this) (room) (was-it) (of) (a) (family) (happy).
The father was a poet and he loved to write during his free time, and
the mother enjoyed all sorts of hobbies.
de ɑdɑ ʌrʌʟ en poet эn ʟuvʌʟ ɥy rytʌl ɑmid ʟλn dΣm otσ. de ʌmɑ ʟuvʌʟ hɔbys
din.
(the)(father)(was-he) (a) (poet) (and)(loved-he)(nes.sent.)(write-he)
(during) (his) (time) (own). (the) (mother) (loved-she) (hobbies)
(many).
I watched as the children came into the room, asking countless
questions with a curious look on their faces.
vysʌm kΣ de kidos kɔmʌv inder de rθm ɥy fʌrʌv din ɑmid ɥy vysʌv ɑn fλsus
fʌrono.
(saw-I) (when) (the) (children) (came-they) (in-loc.) (the) (room)
(nest. sent.) (asked-they) (much) (during)
(nes.sent)(watched-they)(with)(faces) (questioning).
The family loved me, used me, to read their books and papers or play
board games beneath me.
de fʌmʟy ʟuvʌv mu, ʟysʌv vΣn lybres эn pɑpyres ɔf toɥʌv ludos under mu.
(the) (family) (loved-they) (me), (read-they) (their) (books) (and)
(papers) (or) (played-they)(games) (underneath) (me).
The mother used to knit and sew while talking with her friends until
late in the evening, discussing life and asking themselves deliberate
questions.
de ʌmɑ nitenʌʟ эn ȿuωʌʟ ɑmid ɥy sædʌv ɑn ʟλn ɑmys dΣmɑ omэr эnde, diskesʌv
vyvɑ эn fʌrʌv vΣsэv fʌrɑ bɔn.
(the) (mother) (knitted-she) (and) (sewed-she) (during) (nes. sent.)
(talked-they) (with)(her)(friends) (until) (evening) (end),
(discussed-they) (life) (and) (ask-they)(themselves)
(questions)(good).
The conversations were deep and they were engaging their minds to the
fullest. Back then, I wasn't just lighting up a room, I was lighting up
their minds.
de kɔnverɑs ʌrʌv dypfo эn ʌrʌv pэno din. ɑtσw, ʟΣtʌm ne en rθm, ʟΣtʌm vΣn
mΣndes.
(the)(conversations)(were-they)(deep) (and)(were-they)(think) (much).
(Then), (light-I) (not) (a) (room), (light-I) (their) (minds).
I used to watch the father scribble in his notebooks, pondering about
the meaning of life.
vysʌm de ɑdɑ ɥy rytʌʟ inder ʟλn ryt-lybres, pэnʌʟ uber de signyfo vɔn
vyvɑ.
(watched-I) (the)(father)(nes. sent.)(write-he)(in) (his) (notebooks),
(think-he) (about) (the) (meaning) (of) (life).
I would watch the children be so eager to read and explore, and bombard
their parents with endless questions.
vysʌm de kidos ɥy eksytev ɥy ʟysev эn enkɔvev. vΣ fʌrʌv vΣn udэrɑs fʌrɑs
din.
(watch-I)(the)(children)(nes. sent.)(excite-they)(nes.
sent.)(read-they)(and)(explore-they).
(they)(ask-they)(their)(parents)(questions)(many).
But slowly, things started to change.
ɥэt neɥyt, ɑfэros kɔmʌv ɥy ȿʌnsʌv.
(but) (slow), (things) (begin) (nes. sent.) (change-they).
It is not my light that glows over them, but rather a different light,
a steady blue glow illuminating their faces.
ʌreʟ ne mun ȿin-usɔʟ ɥy ȿineʟ vybovder vΣ, ɥэt atσw en usɔʟ nesamo, en
ȿin-usɔʟ kɔnsэkɑ ʟθw ȿineʟ inder vΣn fλsus.
(is-it) (not) (my) (shiny-light)(nes.sent.)(shine-he) (over) (them),
(but) (rather) (a) (light) (different), (a) (shiny-light) (steady)
(blue) (shine-he) (in) (their) (faces).
It started with the children spending less and less time asking
questions, and more time on their screens.
kɔmeʟ ɑn ɥy de kidos fʌrev finfinfinfin fʌrɑ, эn dindin dΣm inder vΣn
ȿin-vytros.
(start-it) (with) (nes.sent.) (the) (children) (ask-they) (less and
less, lesserest) (questions), (and) (more) (time) (in) (their)
(screens).
The father stopped surrounding himself with a bunch of papers and
books, and now brought his laptop with him to the room instead.
de ɑdɑ эndʌʟ ɥy ʌreʟ wisder pɑpyres эn lybres din, эn ɑnσ tэkeʟ ʟyn
kʌʟkuʟɑ vyɑsɑ de rθm ɑn ʟy.
(the) (father) (stopped-he)(nes.sent)(is-he)(between) (papers) (and)
(books) (many), (and)(now) (take-he) (his ) (computer) (to-loc.) (the)
(room)(with)(him).
Finally, the mother now spent more time typing on her little glass
instrument than talking with her friends.
ɑnde, de ʌmɑ ɑnσ ɥθseʟ ʟyn ȿin-vytro dindin dʌn ɥy sædeʟ ɑn ʟyn ɑmys.
(final), (the)(mother) (now) (use-she) (her) (screen) (more) (than)
(nes.sent.) (speak-she)(with)(her) (friends).
As this went on, I noticed the father had stopped writing, the mother
stopped sewing and knitting and the children stopped questioning.
ɑmid dλs, vysʌm de ɑdɑ эndʌʟ ɥy ryteʟ, de ʌmɑ эndʌʟ ɥy ȿuωeʟ эn niteneʟ эn
de kidos эndʌv fʌrono.
(during) (this), (saw-I) (the) (father
(stopped-he)(nes.sent)(write-he), (the) (mother)
(stopped-she)(nes.sent.)(sew-she)(and)(knit-she)(and)(the)(children)
(stopped-they)(questioning).
They also seem to have lost their sense of critical thinking. They were
still the same family, but something had shifted.
ʟθsev ʌʟso vΣn pэna-pσer. эstλhev de fʌmʟy sɑmo, ɥэt ɑfэros ȿʌnsʌʟ.
(lose-they) (also) (their) (think-power). (stay-they) (the) (family)
(same), (but) (things) (changed-it).
I’m getting old. I’m not sure how long I’ll remain glowing.
ʌrɔm odu. wisem ne kɑ dΣm din ɥy эstλhem ɥy ȿinem.
(become-I) (old). (know-I) (not) (how) (time) (much) (nes.sent.)
(stay-I) (nes.sent.) (shine-I).
I’m not even sure if it matters anymore. The light I’m able to deliver
isn’t wanted anymore.
wisem ne ɥy ʌreʟ ɑturo impo. mun usɔʟ wɔʟev ne ɑturo.
(know-I) (not) (nes.sent) (is-it) (anymore) (important). (my) (light)
(want-they) (not) (anymore).
This is a story about something that happened long ago when your grandfather was a child. It is a very important story because it shows how all the comings and goings between our own world and the land of Narnia first began. In those days Mr. Sherlock Holmes was still living in Baker Street and the Bastables were looking for treasure in the Lewisham Road. In those days, if you were a boy you had to wear a stiff Eton collar every day, and schools were usually nastier than now. But meals were nicer; and as for sweets, I won't tell you how cheap and good they were, because it would only make your mouth water in vain. And in those days there lived in London a girl called Polly Plummer. She lived in one of a long row of houses which were all joined together. One morning she was out in the back garden when a boy scrambled up from the garden next door and put his face over the wall. Polly was very surprised because up till now there had never been any children in that house, but only Mr. Ketterley and Miss Ketterley, a brother and sister, old bachelor and old maid, living together. So she looked up, full of curiosity. The face of the strange boy was very grubby. It could hardly have been grubbier if he had first rubbed his hands in the earth, and then had a good cry, and then dried his face with his hands. As a matter of fact, this was very nearly what he had been doing. [...truncated]
dλs ʌreʟ en histɔru uber en ɑfэro ɥy hэpeʟ ɔm ɑtσw din. ɑtσw, dyn odɑ ʌreʟ en kido. ʌreʟ en histɔru impo kʌs ɥy ȿoweʟ ɥy hэpɑs ʌʟ tikse wyn wθd эn de ʟʌnde vɔn Narnia kɔmev эdθn. ɔm dʌts sɔʟes, Σmy Sherlock Holmes hɑbytʌʟ ɑturo эt Baker Street эn de Bastables sukʌm ȿɑsoxu эt de Lewisham Road. ɔm dʌtes sɔʟes, if ʌrʌd en Σmy ʟiʟ, musɔd ɥy wæred en vestθ nevydo ɔm sɔʟ ʌʟ эn ekɔʟes ʌrʌv dindΣm womwom dʌn ɑnσ. ɥэt, eskɑkos ʌrʌv bonbon; эt kæntys, sædem ne ɥy kæntys ʌrʌv kɑ xypuxypu эt kɑ ɥemɥem. kʌs ȿʌʟev ɥy nλɥev dyn ɔro. эn ɔm dʌts sɔʟes, en Σmɑ ʟiʟ nɔmo Polly Plummer hɑbyteʟ эt London. hɑbyteʟ inder en эdrʌn us en row ʌro bσwo ɥono эnsɑmo. ɔm эdrʌn kɔmosɔʟ, ʌrʌʟ inder de hinder-kurɑ kΣ ɥy en Σmy ʟiʟ vyɑsy de nэsder-kurɑ vyseʟ vybovder de wɔʟ. Polly ʌrʌʟ sepryso din kʌs ɥy dΣmɑ nσ, kidos ʌrʌv niʟdΣm inder dʌt bowɑ. ʟono, Σmy Ketterley эn Σmɑ Ketterley, en nybo эn en sis, en Σmy эn en Σmɑ, hɑbytɑv эnsɑmo. dʌs, Polly vysɑʟ vyup ɑn kʌdkuro. The fλs vɔn de nefʌmʟɑ Σmy ʌrʌʟ ȿefugo. kэneʟ ne ȿefugoȿefugo eku ɥy de Σmy tσȿeʟ de эrde mэt ʟyn hʌndes, kyreʟ, эn rλɥeʟ ʟyn fλsu mэt ʟyn hʌndes. afэkto, dʌt ʌrʌʟ finɑ ku hэpo.
P: Gerald! Gerald!
P: Look! Look! xɑ vyse! xɑ vyse!
P: I am invited to a party! ʌrem invyto vyɑsɑ
en fэd.
E: Cool. Σsy.
P: It is cool. ʌreʟ Σsy.
P:
Will you go with me? I have never been to a party.
dɑ gλhɔd ɑn mu? gλhɔm niʟdΣm vyɑsɑ en fэd.
E: I will go with you. I know parties. gλhɔm
ɑn dy. wisem fэdes.
E & P: PARTY! PARTY! PARTY! PARTY! fэd!
fэd! fэd! fэd!
E:
Wait! What if it is a fancy party? We must be ready.
xɑ wλde! dɑ ku if ʌreʟ en fэd fænȿy? musep ɥy ʌrep ʌpo.
P: Really? dɑ?
E: I know parties. wisem fэdes.
P: He knows parties. wiseʟ fэdes.
P: Is this fancy? dɑ dλs ʌreʟ fænȿy?
E: Very fancy. din fænȿy.
E & P: PARTY! PARTY! PARTY! PARTY! fэd!
fэd! fэd! fэd!
E: Wait! What if it is a pool party? xɑ wλde!
dɑ ku if ʌreʟ en fэd ɑkwɑ-ȿʌt?
P: A fancy pool party? dɑ en fэd fænȿy
ɑkwɑ-ȿʌt?
E: WE MUST BE READY!!! musep ɥy ʌrep ʌpo!
E: I know parties. wisem fэdes.
P: He knows parties. wiseʟ fэdes.
P: How is this? dɑ kɑ dλs ʌreʟ?
E: We will make a splash. mλkɔp en эspeʟæȿ.
E & P: PARTY! PARTY! PARTY! PARTY! fэd!
fэd! fэd! fэd!
E: Wait! What if it is a costume party? xɑ
wλde! dɑ ku if ʌreʟ en fэd vestθ?
P: A fancy pool costume party? dɑ en fэd fænȿy
ɑkwɑ-ȿʌt vestθ?
E: WE MUST BE READY!!! musep ɥy ʌrep ʌpo!!!
P: He had better know parties... wiseʟ bonbon
fэdes...
P: Now can we go to the party? ɔm ɑnσw gλhep
vyɑsɑ de fэd?
E: Yes. Now we are ready. ɥæ, ɔm ɑnσw ʌrep
ʌpo.
P:
Well, that is a surprise. You do know parties!
nθ, dλs ʌreʟ en seprys. wised fэdes!
Saj: A Conlang with Two Dimensions of Time. https://youtu.be/DDwf0dorgN8
The Są̂qʌk Language. https://satyrs.eu/saqvk
The Conlanger's Library. https://library.conlang.org
I make languages (and you can too). https://media.ccc.de/v/26c3-3520-en-conlanging_101
Conlanging 101. https://conlang.org/cl101.pdf
The Language Construction Kit. https://www.zompist.com/kit.html
Omniglot. https://omniglot.com
But most important of all, we have hereby scientifically proven that cats are better than dogs. You’re welcome.